By Rachel Morrison
Douglas Goodwin has always been fascinated with how time, context, and perspective shape our conception of reality. His first encounter with this phenomenon occurred when he returned to his hometown of Caldwell, New Jersey (featured in the opening credits of The Sopranos), after a 10-year hiatus. He had grown a foot between the ages of 10 and 20, and when he visited the house where he had spent his childhood, he had a radically different experience of his body in space: whereas once the house had seemed big, now it was impossibly small. 鈥淭his betrayal of my own perception and perceptual system challenged the notion that anything could be objectively 鈥榯rue,鈥欌 Goodwin recalls. Nothing has been the same for him since.
This fall, Goodwin joined the 51猎奇入口 faculty as the first Fletcher Jones Scholar in Computation. A scholar, creative programmer, video artist, and systems architect (he is responsible for the computer networks that undergird the first online university, the University of Colorado鈥檚 Virtual Campus, launched in 1997, as well as the user interface of the Los Angeles Metro), Goodwin is guided in his research by an important question: How do language and other technologies mediate our perception of reality?
Goodwin plans to teach courses that examine the trajectory of photographic technology, including computational photography, as well as alternative and analog 鈥渘on-binary鈥 computation and cybernetics. One of his primary aims, however, is to teach students to develop
a better understanding of cameras and computation and to establish a new relationship to images.
鈥淲e are experiencing a crisis of representation,鈥 explains Goodwin. 鈥淧hotography used to be a simple process of fixing some light in a dark box. Now, some cameras can make pictures entirely by synthetic means.鈥 Consider the current phenomenon of deepfakes: the manipulation of images, audio, and video, via artificial intelligence and machine learning, in order to create footage and photographs of politicians and private citizens engaged in false鈥攁nd oftentimes damning鈥攁cts. While this type of manipulation may seem to be a modern phenomenon, it is in fact as old as image-making itself.
鈥淭here are parallels between these new techniques [for synthetic image-making] and early photographic manipulation,鈥 Goodwin says. Though not all image manipulation is nefarious, it nonetheless has the capacity to undermine the fealty of images as objective representations of reality. To illustrate this point, Goodwin takes readers on a tour of this phenomenon throughout image-making history, showing that to see may not be reason enough to believe.
Here Be Fairies
In 1917, 16-year-old Elsie Wright and nine-year-old Frances Griffiths convinced the British public that they had been visited by fairies. The cousins had produced a series of five photographs that showed the fairies sitting with them near a stream in Cottingley. Although there was a healthy amount of public skepticism, there was also a surprising amount of belief in their veracity, partly because of the British fascination with the supernatural in the wake of World War I. This was bolstered by the fact that photographic manipulation鈥攁nd the ability to spot it鈥攚ere not yet a part of cultural literacy.
鈥淭hese images are not convincing today because we know how to spot this kind of photographic manipulation,鈥 says Goodwin. 鈥淚t鈥檚 easy to see that these fairies are just paper cutouts placed in the foreground of the shot. But although this image may be patently false to our contemporary eyes, we need to continue developing critical looking practices as manipulation techniques and technologies evolve and become more sophisticated.鈥
Fallingwater
Around the same time that Goodwin realized that he had outgrown his childhood conception of home, he traveled to rural Pennsylvania to visit the Frank Lloyd Wright鈥揹esigned house, Fallingwater. 鈥淎s I approached the site, it became clear that I had been duped by all the photographs I had seen prior to coming,鈥 recalls Goodwin. Wright, who stood 5 feet, 7 inches tall, designed Fallingwater to his own proportions. However, according to Goodwin, many promotional photographs depict the home鈥檚 interior spaces as palatial, rather than intimate, in scale. 鈥淭hese images serve more to promote a vision of the architecture and its influence than to describe real space,鈥 Goodwin says. 鈥淭he contradiction suggests propaganda.鈥
Synth or Cynth?
鈥淸Modern] cameras have dispensed with a simple correspondence between subject and image,鈥 says Goodwin. A smartphone photo, for example, is the result of highly orchestrated functions that improve the image, such as motion stabilization, color balance and brightness adjustments, refocusing, and other fixes. 鈥淐onsumers love these features, and our enthusiasm for them clouds our judgment about how mannered and strange these images may appear in 20 years鈥攎uch like the fairy photos from 100 years ago.鈥
Further, as these images proliferate through social networks such as Flickr or Instagram, they enter a data set from which masses of synthetic faces may be created through artificial-intelligence neural networks. 鈥淚 play a game with students called 鈥楽ynth [synthetic] or Cynth [like 鈥淐ynthia鈥漖?鈥 They have to guess which image is synthetic and which is real. It鈥檚 a trick question, though, because both images are synthetic. I want students to develop a critical viewing practice. Are we right to expect so much from photos? To admit them as evidence? And since computers are involved, why do we expect our machines and what they make to be impartial and objective? Because right now, the images pass, and we aren鈥檛 asking the right questions about them.鈥
The images in the top row and far left column are of real people. The inner images are synthetic composites created by StyleGAN. This shows how data sets of real images are used in artificial-intelligence networks to create synthetic images.